Wouldn’t it be great if there was a cheap, non-proprietary,
readily available treatment for patients with heart attacks (acute myocardial
infarction - AMI)? That’s what doctors wanted to believe, so when they saw the
early results of magnesium therapy, they did exactly that. Magnesium therapy
for AMI has been labelled a “lesson
in medical humility”, but I see it as another example of the pervasive bias
amongst researchers, doctors and the public that leads them to overestimate the
effectiveness of medical therapies. Put simply, it was another case of ‘believing
is seeing’.
The lab
The lab
The story starts in the lab where, as expected, magnesium
was shown to have just about every positive effect possible on the heart,
including anti-platelet effects, anti-arrhythmia effects, blood vessel dilation
effects and a reduction in the death of cells in heart attacks. Something this good
in the lab must be good in real life,
right?
The early studies
Early studies on patients showed that those given magnesium
for AMI were less likely to die. Based on these studies, magnesium was widely
recommended and used. Many of the studies were small, but there was one large
study (with over 2,000 patients) which showed a statistically significant
(p=0.03), 2 to 3% difference (reduction) in the rate of death after AMI in
those given magnesium (here
and here).
The larger studies
Later, a study with over 58,000 patients showed no benefit
from magnesium therapy – not just no significant benefit, no benefit at all (here and editorial here).
Due to concerns over that study, a further study with over 6,000 patients was
performed (here),
which also showed no survival benefit from magnesium. Soon after this, in the
early 2000’s, magnesium as a therapy for AMI was dead; magnesium had lost its
mojo.
The explanation
Why did the benefit from magnesium taper off over time (the
‘Decline
Effect’), from outstanding lab and animal studies, and somewhat beneficial
early human studies, to a complete absence of effectiveness?
It is possible that real differences between studies explain
the differences. For example:
1. Animal studies are often not replicated in humans due to
biological differences
2. Early studies may have been performed in patients with
higher risk (therefore having greater benefit), and
3. The later studies may have included other, better
treatments for AMI patients, stealing magnesium’s thunder
These are all reasonable explanations, but so are these:
1. Animal studies are often poor quality, and biased towards
showing and effect that might not exist (see previous blog post here)
2. The smaller, earlier studies are more likely to be biased
towards showing an effect due to poorer methods (here and here, for example)
3. Publication bias may explain why studies showing no
benefit (or harm) from magnesium therapy were not published (as shown here)
The bottom line
Early results showed a small and barely statistically
significant benefit, a benefit that everyone was expecting to see from the lab
and animal studies. The expectation of a result is a strong driver towards
seeing a result, so one possible explanation of the magnesium story is that
biased studies, and biased interpretations of those studies, overestimated the
benefit from magnesium.
And yet daily magnesium has resolved my tachycardia and allowed me to put the bisoprolol in the bin. Perhaps binning the crap levothyroxine also contributed and one wonders if the low T3 of AMI patients isn't relevent here? It's barely tested and even then, rarely treated.
ReplyDeleteLike so many other important nutrients, magnesium is beneficial when obtained from food but not otherwise. Supplements can actually cause deficiencies by interfering with the vitamins and minerals obtained from food. Few researchers seem to understand this.
ReplyDeleteTom, on what basis are you making this claim?
ReplyDelete