Title: Meaning, Medicine and the Placebo Effect (2002)
Author: Daniel Moerman
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
In this book, an anthropologist offers an outsider’s view of
medicine. The book is not restricted to an examination of the placebo effect (in fact, the author suggests abandoning the term, instead using “meaning response”); it asks readers to see all of medicine (and indeed biology) in its social and
cultural context. The author shows that much of what we “know” isn’t
necessarily true (or more confusingly, that it might be true in certain contexts).
In that vein, he criticises doctors for dressing in science (empirical
evidence), but practicing experiential evidence, and therefore not being able
to see that what they “know” (based on tradition and their own experience)
might not be true (despite being able to construct biological mechanisms to explain the perceived effect).
I like reading books about medicine by non-doctors (see book
review: Limits to Medicine); they offer such a refreshing and challenging view
of medicine, and this book is no exception.
Professor Moerman (professor of anthropology based in the
USA) offers an explanation for the placebo effect, starting with the (correct)
observation that placebos, by definition, have no effect. Apart from regression
to the mean and natural history, the placebo effect is best represented by the
term “meaning response”, i.e. the response depends on the meaning perceived by
factors such as the form of the treatment (red pills versus blue pills), the method of delivery (the doctor’s
behaviour and language) and the knowledge surrounding this. And the meaning of
all these factors differs between cultures.
Importantly, the author points out that the meaning response
is largely driven by the doctor delivering the treatment, not the patient
receiving the treatment. He argues that this response is strong and is often
not considered when assessing the effects of medical therapy.
And it is getting harder to measure the true effectiveness
of medicine as the use of placebo controls in research becomes harder to
justify on ethical grounds (although I would argue, and have argued, the
opposite).
The author offers many examples of research that
has been conducted around placebos and the non-specific effects of medicine;
most of these make interesting reading on their own, without the context he
provides. However, he falls short of providing a complete, unifying theory of
the placebo effect, and the overall feeling at the end of the book is that the
meaning we interpret from the act of medicine (form, context, language etc.)
can affect biology, but how much it does so is (perhaps deliberately) left open
to interpretation.
He also lets medicine off the hook occasionally. For
example, he shows how placebos have led to dramatic improvements in cardiac
patients (e.g. angina) over history, calling into question many past
treatments, but then, after pointing out the lack of
placebo testing of current treatments such as bypass surgery, says that we know
it works because we do it much better now. The application of anthropological
and cultural methods (not always amenable to experimental methods) to medicine
is his theme, but I think he should judge scientific claims using scientific
methods. By assuming that treatments work, he is falling for the same trap that
he accuses the doctors of: accepting ‘common knowledge’ as truth.
The bottom line
I believe, as does the author, that doctors need a better
understanding of how much medicine depends on non-specific therapeutic effects
(regression to the mean, natural history and the meaning response) for its
perceived effectiveness, and consequently, how we might be wrong about many
current treatments that we “know” to be effective.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: only a member of this blog may post a comment.